dc.contributor.author
Aryani, Arash
dc.contributor.author
Conrad, Markus
dc.contributor.author
Schmidtke, David
dc.contributor.author
Jacobs, Arthur
dc.date.accessioned
2018-07-04T09:47:27Z
dc.date.available
2018-07-04T09:47:27Z
dc.identifier.uri
https://refubium.fu-berlin.de/handle/fub188/22406
dc.identifier.uri
http://dx.doi.org/10.17169/refubium-215
dc.description.abstract
Most language users agree that some words sound harsh (e.g. grotesque) whereas others sound soft and pleasing (e.g. lagoon). While this prominent feature of human language has always been creatively deployed in art and poetry, it is still largely unknown whether the sound of a word in itself makes any contribution to the word’s meaning as perceived and interpreted by the listener. In a large-scale lexicon analysis, we focused on the affective substrates of words’ meaning (i.e. affective meaning) and words’ sound (i.e. affective sound); both being measured on a two-dimensional space of valence (ranging from pleasant to unpleasant) and arousal (ranging from calm to excited). We tested the hypothesis that the sound of a word possesses affective iconic characteristics that can implicitly influence listeners when evaluating the affective meaning of that word. The results show that a significant portion of the variance in affective meaning ratings of printed words depends on a number of spectral and temporal acoustic features extracted from these words after converting them to their spoken form (study1). In order to test the affective nature of this effect, we independently assessed the affective sound of these words using two different methods: through direct rating (study2a), and through acoustic models that we implemented based on pseudoword materials (study2b). In line with our hypothesis, the estimated contribution of words’ sound to ratings of words’ affective meaning was indeed associated with the affective sound of these words; with a stronger effect for arousal than for valence. Further analyses revealed crucial phonetic features potentially causing the effect of sound on meaning: For instance, words with short vowels, voiceless consonants, and hissing sibilants (as in ‘piss’) feel more arousing and negative. Our findings suggest that the process of meaning making is not solely determined by arbitrary mappings between formal aspects of words and concepts they refer to. Rather, even in silent reading, words’ acoustic profiles provide affective perceptual cues that language users may implicitly use to construct words’ overall meaning.
en
dc.format.extent
27 Seiten
de
dc.rights.uri
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
de
dc.subject
words’ sound
en
dc.subject
iconic characteristics
de
dc.subject.ddc
400 Sprache::410 Linguistik::414 Phonologie, Phonetik
de
dc.title
Why 'piss' is ruder than 'pee'? The role of sound in affective meaning making
de
dc.type
Wissenschaftlicher Artikel
de
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.articlenumber
e0198430
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.doi
10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0198430
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.journaltitle
PLoS ONE
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.number
6
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.volume
13
dcterms.bibliographicCitation.url
https://doi.org/10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0198430
de
refubium.affiliation
Erziehungswissenschaft und Psychologie
de
refubium.affiliation.other
Arbeitsbereich Allgemeine und Neurokognitive Psychologie

de
refubium.funding
Institutional Participation
refubium.note.author
Die Publikation wurde aus Open Access Publikationsgeldern der Freien Universität Berlin und der DFG gefördert.
de
refubium.resourceType.isindependentpub
no
de
dcterms.accessRights.openaire
open access